[صفحه اصلی ]   [Archive] [ English ]  
:: صفحه اصلي :: درباره نشريه :: آخرين شماره :: تمام شماره‌ها :: جستجو :: ثبت نام :: ارسال مقاله ::
بخش‌های اصلی
صفحه اصلی::
اطلاعات نشریه::
آرشیو مجله و مقالات::
برای نویسندگان::
برای داوران::
اصول اخلاقی::
ثبت نام و اشتراک::
تسهیلات پایگاه::
تماس با ما::
::
::
جستجو در پایگاه

جستجوی پیشرفته
..
دریافت اطلاعات پایگاه
نشانی پست الکترونیک خود را برای دریافت اطلاعات و اخبار پایگاه، در کادر زیر وارد کنید.
..
Copyright Policies

AWT IMAGE

..
Open Access Policy

نحوه دسترسی به تمام مقالات مجله بصورت زیر است:

Creative Commons License
..

..
ثبت شده در

AWT IMAGE

AWT IMAGE

..
:: دوره 6، شماره 3 - ( تابستان - 1397 ) ::
دوره 6 شماره 3 صفحات 90-79 برگشت به فهرست نسخه ها
نظریه‌های کنترل حرکتی: ارائه یک مدل ساختاری یکپارچه بر اساس مفاهیم مشترک
مصطفی تیموری خروی ، علیرضا صابری کاخکی* ، محمد دارینی ، سارا سرحدی ، زینب رستگار
گروه رفتار حرکتی، دانشگاه فردوسی مشهد، مشهد، ایران
چکیده:   (5720 مشاهده)
مقدمه: مطالعات بسیاری بر روی مکانیسم کنترل و یادگیری حرکتی انجام شده که منجر به نظریه‌های متعددی در این زمینه شده است. هدف از مطالعه حاضر بررسی این نظریه‌ها و یکپارچه کردن آن‌ها در قالب یک مدل مفهومی جهت درک بهتر کنترل و یادگیری حرکتی بود. نتیجه‌گیری: برخی از مطالعات کنترل حرکتی نشان داده است که اکثر حرکات به‌ویژه حرکات دسترسی که به دقت بالایی نیاز دارند، شامل دو بخش افزایش شتاب و کاهش شتاب می‌باشند. مرور برخی از نظریه‌های کنترل حرکتی نشان می‌دهد که آن‌ها شامل دو بخش هستند که هر یک از آن‌ها بخش‌ خاصی از حرکت دسترسی را به صورت جداگانه و با مکانیسم‌های متفاوت کنترل می‌کند. یکپارچه کردن این نظریه‌ها بر اساس ساختارهای عصبی درگیر در هر مرحله یک درک جامع از چگونگی کنترل حرکتی فراهم می‌کند. ما مدل یکپارچه مفهومی را پیشنهاد می‌کنیم.
واژه‌های کلیدی: مغز، برتری یافتگی کارکردی، حرکت، عملکرد روانشناختی حرکتی
متن کامل [PDF 716 kb]   (19904 دریافت)    
نوع مطالعه: مروری | موضوع مقاله: روانشناسی
فهرست منابع
1. Yavari F, Towhidkhah F, Ahmadi-Pajouh MA. Are fast/slow process in motor adaptation and forward/inverse internal model two sides of the same coin? Med Hypotheses. 2013; 81(4): 592-600. [DOI:10.1016/j.mehy.2013.07.009]
2. Yadav V, Sainburg RL. Handedness can be explained by a serial hybrid control scheme. Neuroscience. 2014; 278: 385-96. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.08.026]
3. Todorov E. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat Neurosci. 2004; 7(9): 907-15. [DOI:10.1038/nn1309]
4. Desmurget M, Grafton S. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000; 4(11): 423-31. [DOI:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01537-0]
5. Milner TE. A model for the generation of movements requiring endpoint precision. Neuroscience. 1992; 49(2): 487-96. [DOI:10.1016/0306-4522(92)90113-G]
6. Jeannerod M. The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements: Oxford University Press. 1988. p. 296.
7. Carlton LG. Processing visual feedback information for movement control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1981; 7(5): 1019-30. [DOI:10.1037/0096-1523.7.5.1019]
8. Beaubaton D, Hay L. Contribution of visual information to feedforward and feedback processes in rapid pointing movements. Hum Mov Sci. 1986; 5(1): 19-34. [DOI:10.1016/0167-9457(86)90003-5]
9. Stroeve S. A learning feedback and feedforward neuromuscular control model for two degrees of freedom human arm movements. Hum Mov Sci. 1997; 16(5): 621-51. [DOI:10.1016/S0167-9457(97)00009-2]
10. Rosa M. Co-contraction role on human motor control. a neural basis. Journal of Novel Physiotherapies. 2015; 2015.
11. Bagesteiro LB, Sainburg RL. Nondominant arm advantages in load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements. J Neurophysiol. 2003; 90(3): 1503-13. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00189.2003]
12. Haaland KY, Prestopnik JL, Knight RT, Lee RR. Hemispheric asymmetries for kinematic and positional aspects of reaching. Brain. 2004; 127(5): 1145-58. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awh133]
13. Jordan MI, Rumelhart DE. Forward models: Supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cogn Sci. 1992; 16(3): 307-54. [DOI:10.1207/s15516709cog1603_1]
14. Mehta B, Schaal S. Forward models in visuomotor control. J Neurophysiol. 2002; 88(2): 942-53. [DOI:10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.942]
15. Kawato M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999; 9(6): 718-27. [DOI:10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8]
16. Tseng Y-w, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 98(1): 54-62. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00266.2007]
17. Porrill J, Dean P, Anderson SR. Adaptive filters and internal models: multilevel description of cerebellar function. Neural Netw. 2013; 47: 134-49. [DOI:10.1016/j.neunet.2012.12.005]
18. Bastian AJ. Moving, sensing and learning with cerebellar damage. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011; 21(4): 596-601. [DOI:10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.007]
19. Cheng K-H. Adaptive fuzzy CMAC-based nonlinear control with dynamic memory architecture. J Franklin Inst. 2011; 348(9): 2480-502. [DOI:10.1016/j.jfranklin.2011.07.011]
20. Max L, Guenther FH, Gracco VL, Ghosh SS, Wallace ME. Unstable or insufficiently activated internal models and feedback-biased motor control as sources of dysfluency: a theoretical model of stuttering. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord. 2004; 31: 105-22.
21. Dindo H, Zambuto D, Pezzulo G. Motor simulation via coupled internal models using sequential monte carlo. IJCAI Proceedings-International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence; 2011. 2113-9.
22. Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR. Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011; 12(12): 739-51. [DOI:10.1038/nrn3112]
23. Feys P, Helsen W, Nuttin B, Lavrysen A, Ketelaer P, Swinnen S, et al. Unsteady gaze fixation enhances the severity of MS intention tremor. Neurology. 2008; 70(2): 106-13. [DOI:10.1212/01.wnl.0000287071.51180.b1]
24. Salmoni AW, Schmidt RA, Walter CB. Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. Psychol Bull. 1984; 95(3): 355. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.355]
25. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor learning and control. 1988.
26. Schmidt RA, White L. Evidence for an error detection mechanism in motor skills: a test of Adams' closed-loop theory. J Mot Behav. 1972: 4(3); 143-53. [DOI:10.1080/00222895.1972.10734930]
27. Newell KM. Schema theory (1975): retrospectives and prospectives. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2003; 74(4): 383-8. [DOI:10.1080/02701367.2003.10609108]
28. Sherwood D, Lee T. Cognitive effort and schema theory: Implications for a new theory of motor learning. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2003; 74: 376-82. [DOI:10.1080/02701367.2003.10609107]
29. Schmidt RA. Motor control and learning . hu man kinetics Publishers. Inc. Champaign, Illinois. 1988: p. 105-16.
30. Schmidt RA, White JL. Evidence for an error detection mechanism in motor skills: a test of Adams' closed-loop theory. J Mot Behav. 1972; 4(3): 143-53. [DOI:10.1080/00222895.1972.10734930]
31. Nicholson DE, Schmidt RA. Timing-task duration determines post-response error-detection capabilities. North American society for psychology of sport and physical activity annual meeting, Monterey, CA; 1991.
32. Coelho CJ, Przybyla A, Yadav V, Sainburg RL. Hemispheric differences in the control of limb dynamics: a link between arm performance asymmetries and arm selection patterns. J Neurophysiol. 2013; 109(3): 825-38. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00885.2012]
33. Schaefer SY, Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Hemispheric specialization for movement control produces dissociable differences in online corrections after stroke. Cereb Cortex. 2012; 22(6): 1407-19. [DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhr237]
34. Oliveira FT, Diedrichsen J, Verstynen T, Duque J, Ivry RB. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of posterior parietal cortex affects decisions of hand choice. PNAS. 2010; 107(41): 17751-6. [DOI:10.1073/pnas.1006223107]
35. Teymuri M, Saberi Kakhki A, Taheri HR, Ghanaei CHamanabad A, Darainy M. The effect of brain hemisphere stimulation and how to specialize motor task programming: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. Journal of Neurological Sciences. 2017; 34(3).
36. Kheravi MT, Kakhki AS, Taheri H, Chamanabad AG, Darainy M. Researching hemispheric specialization in the control of final position accuracy using the transcranial direct current stimulation. Glob J Health Sci. 2017; 9(7): 138-49. [DOI:10.5539/gjhs.v9n7p138]
37. Teymuri M, Saberi Kakhki A, Taheri HR, Ghanaei CHamanabad A, Darainy M. The use of direct current stimulation to investigate the role of each hemisphere in motor learning of reaching task. Shefayekhatam. 2017; 5(4): 65-75.
38. Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY. Left parietal regions are critical for adaptive visuomotor control. J Neurosci. 2011; 31(19): 6972-81. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6432-10.2011]
39. Zwinkels A, Geusgens C, Van de Sande P, Van Heugten C. Assessment of apraxia: inter-rater reliability of a new apraxia test, association between apraxia and other cognitive deficits and prevalence of apraxia in a rehabilitation setting. Clin Rehabil. 2004; 18(7): 819-27. [DOI:10.1191/0269215504cr816oa]
40. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke. Brain Res. 2009; 1298: 78-91. [DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.063]
41. Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. The effects of brain lateralization on motor control and adaptation. J Mot Behav. 2012; 44(6): 455-69. [DOI:10.1080/00222895.2012.747482]
42. Stöckel T, Weigelt M. Brain lateralisation and motor learning: selective effects of dominant and non-dominant hand practice on the early acquisition of throwing skills. Laterality. 2012; 17(1): 18-37. [DOI:10.1080/1357650X.2010.524222]
43. Schambra HM, Abe M, Luckenbaugh DA, Reis J, Krakauer JW, Cohen LG. Probing for hemispheric specialization for motor skill learning: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. J Neurophysiol. 2011; 106(2): 652-61. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00210.2011]
44. Stöckel T, Wang J. Transfer of short-term motor learning across the lower limbs as a function of task conception and practice order. Brain Cogn. 2011; 77(2): 271-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.010]
45. Senff O, Weigelt M. Sequential effects after practice with the dominant and non-dominant hand on the acquisition of a sliding task in schoolchildren. Laterality. 2011; 16(2): 227-39. [DOI:10.1080/13576500903549414]
46. Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY. Critical neural substrates for correcting unexpected trajectory errors and learning from them. Brain. 2011; 134(12): 3647-61. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awr275]
47. Duff SV, Sainburg RL. Lateralization of motor adaptation reveals independence in control of trajectory and steady-state position. Exp Brain Res. 2007; 179(4): 551-61. [DOI:10.1007/s00221-006-0811-1]
48. Schabowsky CN, Hidler JM, Lum PS. Greater reliance on impedance control in the nondominant arm compared with the dominant arm when adapting to a novel dynamic environment. Exp Brain Res. 2007; 182(4): 567-77. [DOI:10.1007/s00221-007-1017-x]
49. Goble DJ, Brown SH. Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. J Neurophysiol. 2008; 99(6): 3063-74. [DOI:10.1152/jn.90259.2008]
50. Sainburg RL. Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2005; 33(4): 206-13. [DOI:10.1097/00003677-200510000-00010]
51. Rosenbaum DA. Human motor control. 2nd ed. Academic press. 2009. p. 528.
52. Schmidt R, Lee T. Motor Learning and performance, 5th ed with web study guide. Human Kinetics. 2014; 336.
53. Sainburg RL, Schaefer SY. Interlimb differences in control of movement extent. J Neurophysiol. 2004; 92(3): 1374-83. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00181.2004]
54. Grafton ST, Desmurget M, Alexander G. Functional anatomy of error correction during pointing without vision of the hand. Ann Neurol 1999; 46(3): 447.
55. Inoue K, Kawashima R, Satoh K, Kinomura S, Goto R, Koyama M, et al. PET study of pointing with visual feedback of moving hands. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 79(1): 117-25. [DOI:10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.117]
56. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect hemispheric specializations for movement control. Brain. 2007; 130(8): 2146-58. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awm145]
57. Anguera JA, Russell CA, Noll DC, Seidler RD. Neural correlates associated with intermanual transfer of sensorimotor adaptation. Brain Res. 2007; 1185: 136-51. [DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.088]
58. Serrien DJ, Ivry RB, Swinnen SP. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006; 7(2): 160-6. [DOI:10.1038/nrn1849]
59. Goldenberg G. Apraxia and beyond: life and work of Hugo Liepmann. Cortex. 2003; 39(3): 509-24. [DOI:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70261-2]
60. Pearce J. Hugo Karl Liepmann and apraxia. Clin Med (Lond). 2009; 9(5): 466-70. [DOI:10.7861/clinmedicine.9-5-466]
61. Hollerbach JM. Computers, brains, and the control of movement. Trends Neurosci. 1982; 5: 1-474. [DOI:10.1016/0166-2236(82)90111-4]
62. Gerdes V, Happee R. The use of an internal representation in fast goal-directed movements: a modelling approach. Biol Cybern. 1994; 70(6): 513-24. [DOI:10.1007/BF00198804]
63. Diedrichsen J, White O, Newman D, Lally N. Use-dependent and error-based learning of motor behaviors. J Neurosci. 2010; 30(15): 5159-66. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5406-09.2010]
64. Verstynen T, Sabes PN. How each movement changes the next: an experimental and theoretical study of fast adaptive priors in reaching. J Neurosci. 2011; 31(27): 10050-9. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6525-10.2011]
65. Bonato M. Neglect and extinction depend greatly on task demands: a review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012; 6: 195. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00195. [DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00195]
66. Stöttinger E, Filipowicz A, Marandi E, Quehl N, Danckert J, Anderson B. Statistical and perceptual updating: correlated impairments in right brain injury. Exp Brain Res. 2014; 232(6): 1971-87. [DOI:10.1007/s00221-014-3887-z]
67. Shaqiri A, Anderson B. Priming and statistical learning in right brain damaged patients. Neuropsychologia. 2013; 51(13): 2526-33. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.024]
68. Shaqiri A, Anderson B, Danckert J. Statistical learning as a tool for rehabilitation in spatial neglect. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7: 224. [DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00224]
69. Vocat R, Saj A, Vuilleumier P. The riddle of anosognosia: does unawareness of hemiplegia involve a failure to update beliefs? Cortex. 2013; 49(7): 1771-81. [DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.009]



XML   English Abstract   Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Teymuri Kheravi M, Saberi Kakhki A, Darainy M, Sarhadi S, Rastegar Z. Motor Control Theories: Providing an Integrated Structural Model Based on Common Concepts. Shefaye Khatam 2018; 6 (3) :79-90
URL: http://shefayekhatam.ir/article-1-1739-fa.html

تیموری خروی مصطفی، صابری کاخکی علیرضا، دارینی محمد، سرحدی سارا، رستگار زینب. نظریه‌های کنترل حرکتی: ارائه یک مدل ساختاری یکپارچه بر اساس مفاهیم مشترک. مجله علوم اعصاب شفای خاتم. 1397; 6 (3) :79-90

URL: http://shefayekhatam.ir/article-1-1739-fa.html



بازنشر اطلاعات
Creative Commons License این مقاله تحت شرایط Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License قابل بازنشر است.
دوره 6، شماره 3 - ( تابستان - 1397 ) برگشت به فهرست نسخه ها
مجله علوم اعصاب شفای خاتم The Neuroscience Journal of Shefaye Khatam
Persian site map - English site map - Created in 0.05 seconds with 53 queries by YEKTAWEB 4645